More threads by David Baxter PhD

David Baxter PhD

Late Founder
Scientists Discover The Part Of The Brain That Causes Some People To Be Lousy In Math
March 22, 2006
California Institute Of Technology

Most everyone knows that the term "dyslexia" refers to people who can't keep words and letters straight. A rarer term is "dyscalculia," which describes someone who is virtually unable to deal with numbers, much less do complicated math.

Scientists now have discovered the area of the brain linked to dyscalculia, demonstrating that there is a specific part of the brain essential for counting properly. In a report published in the March 13 issue of the Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (PNAS), researchers explain that the area of the brain known as the intraparietal sulcus (IPS), located toward the top and back of the brain and across both lobes, is crucial for the proper processing of numerical information.

According to Fulvia Castelli, a postdoctoral researcher at the California Institute of Technology and lead author of the paper, the IPS has been known for years as the brain area that allows humans to conceive of numbers. But she and her coauthors from University College London demonstrate that the IPS specifically determines how many things are perceived, as opposed to how much.

To explain how intimately the two different modes of thinking are connected, Castelli says to think about what happens when a person is approaching the checkout lines at the local Trader Joe's. Most of us are impatient sorts, so we typically head for the shortest line.

"Imagine how you really pick the shortest checkout line," says Castelli. "You could count the number of shoppers in each line, in which case you'd be thinking discretely in terms of numerosity.

"But if you're a hurried shopper, you probably take a quick glance at each line and pick the one that seems the shortest. In this case you're thinking in terms of continuous quantity."

The two modes of thinking are so similar, in fact, that scientists have had trouble isolating specific networks within the IPS because it is very difficult to distinguish between responses of how many and how much. To get at the difference between the two forms of quantity processing, Castelli and her colleagues devised a test in which subjects performed quick estimations of quantity while under functional MRI scans.

Specifically, the researchers showed subjects a series of blue and green flashes of light or a chessboard with blue and green rectangles. The subjects were asked to judge whether they saw more green or more blue, and their brain activity was monitored while they did so.

The results show that while subjects are exposed to the separate colors, the brain automatically counts how many objects are present. However, when subjects are presented with either a continuous blue and green light or a blurred chessboard on which the single squares are no longer visible, the brain does not count the objects, but instead estimates how much blue and green is visible.

"We think this identifies the brain activity specific to estimating the number of things," Castelli says. "This is probably also a brain network that underlies arithmetic, and when it's abnormal, may be responsible for dyscalculia."

In other words, dyscalculia arises because a person cannot develop adequate representations of how many things there are.

"Of course, dyscalculics can learn to count," Castelli explains. "But where most people can immediately tell that nine is bigger than seven, anyone with dcyscalculia may have to count the objects to be sure.

"Similarly, dyscalculics are much slower than people in general when they have to say how many objects there are in a set," she adds. "This affects everyday life, from the time when a child is struggling to keep up with arithmetic lessons in school to the time when an adult is trying to deal with money."

The good news is that the work of Castelli and her colleagues could lead to better tools for assessing whether a learning technique for people with dyscalculia is actually working. "Now that we have identified the brain system that carries out this function, we are in a position to see how dyscalculic brain activities differ from a normal brain," Castelli says.

"We should be in a position to measure whether an intervention is changing the brain function so that it becomes more like the normal pattern."

The article is titled Discrete and analogue quantity processing in the parietal lobe: A functional MRI study. Castelli's coauthors are Daniel E. Glaser and Brian Butterworth, both researchers at the Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience at University College London.
 

Similar threads

[Brain"]Queen's Gazette | Queen's University Areas Identified That 'Decode' Emotions Of Others[/url] 2004-04-16 Kingston, ON -- Queen's University psychologists have discovered that our ability to assess how other people are feeling relies on two specific areas of the brain. The findings, published in the April issue of the Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, are expected to have implications for the treatment of developmental disorders such as autism. Led by Mark Sabbagh, the study is...
Replies
0
Views
2K
https://www.psychologytoday.com/us/blog/your-brain-food/202211/how-blueberries-may-help-reduce-brain-fog by Gary Wenk Ph.D. November 2, 2022 Recent studies have discovered specific changes in brain function. Daily consumption of the polyphenols found in blueberries may effectively slow cognitive decline. Consuming blueberries improved the speed of processing, which is the time required to complete a mental task. Daily blueberry consumption also significantly protected against cognitive...
Replies
0
Views
86
What Is Brain Fog and How Can I Treat It? by Knvul Sheikh, The New York Times Sept 13, 2022 Researchers are just beginning to understand the cognitive dysfunction that some people experience with Covid-19 and a range of other health issues. Q: I have a hard time remembering things and often feel exhausted, like I can’t clear my head at all. Is this brain fog, and is there anything I can do to fix it? The blank space in your mind, when it appears, can be confusing. What were you just...
Replies
0
Views
160
How Social Anxiety Affects the Brain Psych Central Sept 7, 2022 Research shows changes in important areas of the brain, such as the hippocampus and amygdala, can be affected by social anxiety. Social anxiety disorder, also known as social phobia, is a form of anxiety that causes intense fear and embarrassment in social situations. For example, it’s common to feel slightly nervous when meeting new people or speaking in public. People with social anxiety disorder can experience a paralyzing...
Replies
0
Views
1K
Why Daydreaming Is Actually Good For Your Brain by Melinda Wenner Moyer, The New York Times August 4, 2022 Whenever I have a few moments of down time — every weekday, for instance, when I’m waiting in the car pool pickup line for my children at camp — I grab my phone and check to see whether anything interesting has happened on Instagram. The thing is, I don’t particularly like Instagram. Social media usually makes me feel insecure, but somehow that is preferable to sitting alone with my...
Replies
0
Views
151
Top