David Baxter PhD
Late Founder
The Gregarious Brain
By DAVID DOBBS, New York Times
July 8, 2007
If a person suffers the small genetic accident that creates Williams syndrome, he?ll live with not only some fairly conventional cognitive deficits, like trouble with space and numbers, but also a strange set of traits that researchers call the Williams social phenotype or, less formally, the ?Williams personality?: a love of company and conversation combined, often awkwardly, with a poor understanding of social dynamics and a lack of social inhibition. The combination creates some memorable encounters. Oliver Sacks, the neurologist and author, once watched as a particularly charming 8-year-old Williams girl, who was visiting Sacks at his hotel, took a garrulous detour into a wedding ceremony. ?I?m afraid she disrupted the flow of this wedding,? Sacks told me. ?She also mistook the bride?s mother for the bride. That was an awkward moment. But it very much pleased the mother.?
Another Williams encounter: The mother of twin Williams boys in their late teens opened her door to find on her stoop a leather-clad biker, motorcycle parked at the curb, asking for her sons. The boys had made the biker?s acquaintance via C.B. radio and invited him to come by, but they forgot to tell Mom. The biker visited for a spell. Fascinated with how the twins talked about their condition, the biker asked them to speak at his motorcycle club?s next meeting. They did. They told the group of the genetic accident underlying Williams, the heart and vascular problems that eventually kill many who have it, their intense enjoyment of talk, music and story, their frustration in trying to make friends, the slights and cruelties they suffered growing up, their difficulty understanding the world. When they finished, most of the bikers were in tears.
These stories are typical of those who have Williams syndrome. (Some people with the disorder as well as many who work with them simply call it Williams.) Williams syndrome rises from a genetic accident during meiosis, when DNA?s double helix is divided into two separate strands, each strand then becoming the genetic material in egg or sperm. Normally the two strands part cleanly, like a zipper?s two halves. But in Williams, about 25 teeth in one of the zippers ? 25 genes out of 30,000 in egg or sperm ? are torn loose during this parting. When that strand joins another from the other parent to eventually form an embryo, the segment of the DNA missing those 25 genes can?t do its work.
The resulting cognitive deficits lie mainly in the realm of abstract thought. Many with Williams have so vague a concept of space, for instance, that even as adults they will fail at six-piece jigsaw puzzles, easily get lost, draw like a preschooler and struggle to replicate a simple T or X shape built with a half-dozen building blocks. Few can balance a checkbook. These deficits generally erase about 35 points from whatever I.Q. the person would have inherited without the deletion. Since the average I.Q. is 100, this leaves most people with Williams with I.Q.?s in the 60s. Though some can hold simple jobs, they require assistance managing their lives.
The low I.Q., however, ignores two traits that define Williams more distinctly than do its deficits: an exuberant gregariousness and near-normal language skills. Williams people talk a lot, and they talk with pretty much anyone. They appear to truly lack social fear. Indeed, functional brain scans have shown that the brain?s main fear processor, the amygdala, which in most of us shows heightened activity when we see angry or worried faces, shows no reaction when a person with Williams views such faces. It?s as if they see all faces as friendly.
People with Williams tend to lack not just social fear but also social savvy. Lost on them are many meanings, machinations, ideas and intentions that most of us infer from facial expression, body language, context and stock phrasings. If you?re talking with someone with Williams syndrome and look at your watch and say: ?Oh, my, look at the time! Well it?s been awfully nice talking with you . . . ,? your conversational partner may well smile brightly, agree that ?this is nice? and ask if you?ve ever gone to Disney World. Because of this ? and because many of us feel uneasy with people with cognitive disorders, or for that matter with anyone profoundly unlike us ? people with Williams can have trouble deepening relationships. This saddens and frustrates them. They know no strangers but can claim few friends.
This paradox ? the urge to connect, the inability to fully do so ? sits at the center of the Williams puzzle, whether considered as a picture of human need (who hasn?t been shut out of a circle he?d like to join?) or, as a growing number of researchers are finding, a clue to the fundamental drives and tensions that shape social behavior. After being ignored for almost three decades, Williams has recently become one of the most energetically researched neurodevelopmental disability after autism, and it is producing more compelling insights. Autism, for starters, is a highly diverse ?spectrum disorder? with ill-defined borders, no identified mechanism and no clearly delineated genetic basis. Williams, in contrast, arises from a known genetic cause and produces a predictable set of traits and behaviors. It is ?an experiment of nature,? as the title of one paper puts it, perfect for studying not just how genes create intelligence and sociability but also how our powers of thought combine with our desire to bond to create complex social behavior ? a huge arena of interaction that largely determines our fates.
Julie R. Korenberg, a neurogeneticist at Cedars-Sinai Medical Center and at the University of California, Los Angeles, who has helped define the Williams deletion and explore its effects, believes the value of Williams syndrome in examining such questions is almost impossible to overstate. ?We?ve long figured that major behavioral traits rose in indirect fashion from a wide array of genes,? Korenberg says. ?But here we have this really tiny genetic deletion ? of the 20-some-odd genes missing, probably just 3 to 6 create the cognitive and social effects ? that reliably creates a distinctive behavioral profile. Williams isn?t just a fascinating mix of traits. It is the most compelling model available for studying the genetic bases of human behavior.?
Korenberg?s work is part of a diverse research effort on Williams that is illuminating a central dilemma of human existence: to survive we must relate and work with others, but we must also compete against them, lest we get left behind. It?s like the TV show ?Survivor?: we want to keep a place in the group ? we must ? and doing so requires not only charming others but also showing we can contribute to their success. This requires a finely calibrated display of smarts, savvy, grit and hustle. Show too little, and you?re voted off the island for being subpar. Show too much, and you?re ousted as a conniving threat.
Where is the right balance? A partial answer lies in the mix of skills, charms and deficiencies that is Williams syndrome.
Williams syndrome was first identified in 1961 by Dr. J. C. P. Williams of New Zealand. Williams, a cardiologist at Greenlane Hospital in Auckland, noticed that a number of the hospital?s young cardiac patients were small in stature, had elfin facial features and seemed friendly but in some ways were mentally slow. His published delineation of this syndrome put Dr. Williams on the map ? off which he promptly and mysteriously fell. Twice offered a position at the prestigious Mayo Clinic in Rochester, Minn., he twice failed to show, disappearing the second time, in the late ?60s, from London, his last known location, with the only trace an unclaimed suitcase later found in a luggage office.
The rarity of Williams syndrome ? about 1 in 7,500 people have it, compared with about 1 in 150 for autism or 1 in 800 for Down syndrome ? rendered it obscure. Unless they had the syndrome?s distinctive cardiovascular problems (which stem from the absence of the gene that makes blood vessels, heart valves and other tissue elastic and which even today limit the average lifespan of a person with Williams to around 50), most people with Williams were simply considered ?mentally retarded.?
This ended in the late 1980s, when a few researchers in the emerging field of cognitive neuroscience began to explore Williams. Among the most earnest was Ursula Bellugi, the director of the Laboratory for Cognitive Neuroscience at the Salk Institute for Biological Studies in La Jolla, Calif. Bellugi, who specializes in the neurobiology of language, was drawn to the linguistic strength that many Williamses displayed in the face of serious cognitive problems. The first person with Williams she met, in fact, came by referral from the linguist Noam Chomsky.
?The mother of that Williams teenager later connected me with two more, both in their teens,? Bellugi said. ?I didn?t have to talk to them long to realize something special was going on. Here they had these great cognitive deficits. Yet they spoke with the most ardent and delightful animation and color.?
To understand this uneven cognitive profile, Bellugi gave an array of language and cognitive tests to three groups: Williams children and teenagers, Down syndrome kids with similar I.Q.?s and developmentally average peers. ?We would do these warm-up interviews to get to know them, ask about their families,? said Bellugi, who, less than five feet tall and with a ready smile and an animated manner, is somewhat elfin and engagingly gregarious herself. ?Only, the Williams kids would turn the tables. They?d tell you how pretty you look or ask, ?Do you like opera?? They would ornament their answers in a way other kids didn?t. For instance, you?d ask an adolescent, ?What if you were a bird?? The Down kids said things like: ?I?m not a bird. I don?t fly.? The Williams teens would say: ?Good question! I?d fly through the air being free. If I saw a boy I?d land on his head and chirp.? ?
Bellugi found that this fanciful verbosity was accompanied by infectious affability. To measure it she developed a questionnaire and gave it to parents of Williams, Down and normal children. It asked about things like friendliness toward strangers, connections to familiar people, different social scenarios. At every age level, those with Williams scored significantly higher in sociability than those in the other groups. Having long studied the human capacity for language and its biological basis, Bellugi assumed that some extraordinary urge to use language drove this hypersociability: ?The language just seemed to be erupting out of them.?
Then she attended a meeting of Williams families that included infants and toddlers. ?That was about a year into my research project,? she says. ?The room was full of little ones ? babies, toddlers who weren?t speaking yet. And when I came in the room all the young children old enough to walk ran to the door to greet me. No clinging to Mom; they just broke away. And when I would talk to mothers holding infants ? literally babes in arms ? some of these babies would almost dive out of their mothers? arms to meet me.
?I knew then I was wrong. The language wasn?t driving the sociability. If anything, it was the other way around.?
Developmental psychologists sometimes call the social urge the ?drive to affiliate.? It seemed clear early on that the Williams deletion, which was definitively identified in the mid-1990s, either strengthened this drive or left it unfettered. But how do missing genes steer behavior toward gregariousness and engagement? How can a deletion heighten a trait rather than diminish it?
By DAVID DOBBS, New York Times
July 8, 2007
If a person suffers the small genetic accident that creates Williams syndrome, he?ll live with not only some fairly conventional cognitive deficits, like trouble with space and numbers, but also a strange set of traits that researchers call the Williams social phenotype or, less formally, the ?Williams personality?: a love of company and conversation combined, often awkwardly, with a poor understanding of social dynamics and a lack of social inhibition. The combination creates some memorable encounters. Oliver Sacks, the neurologist and author, once watched as a particularly charming 8-year-old Williams girl, who was visiting Sacks at his hotel, took a garrulous detour into a wedding ceremony. ?I?m afraid she disrupted the flow of this wedding,? Sacks told me. ?She also mistook the bride?s mother for the bride. That was an awkward moment. But it very much pleased the mother.?
Another Williams encounter: The mother of twin Williams boys in their late teens opened her door to find on her stoop a leather-clad biker, motorcycle parked at the curb, asking for her sons. The boys had made the biker?s acquaintance via C.B. radio and invited him to come by, but they forgot to tell Mom. The biker visited for a spell. Fascinated with how the twins talked about their condition, the biker asked them to speak at his motorcycle club?s next meeting. They did. They told the group of the genetic accident underlying Williams, the heart and vascular problems that eventually kill many who have it, their intense enjoyment of talk, music and story, their frustration in trying to make friends, the slights and cruelties they suffered growing up, their difficulty understanding the world. When they finished, most of the bikers were in tears.
These stories are typical of those who have Williams syndrome. (Some people with the disorder as well as many who work with them simply call it Williams.) Williams syndrome rises from a genetic accident during meiosis, when DNA?s double helix is divided into two separate strands, each strand then becoming the genetic material in egg or sperm. Normally the two strands part cleanly, like a zipper?s two halves. But in Williams, about 25 teeth in one of the zippers ? 25 genes out of 30,000 in egg or sperm ? are torn loose during this parting. When that strand joins another from the other parent to eventually form an embryo, the segment of the DNA missing those 25 genes can?t do its work.
The resulting cognitive deficits lie mainly in the realm of abstract thought. Many with Williams have so vague a concept of space, for instance, that even as adults they will fail at six-piece jigsaw puzzles, easily get lost, draw like a preschooler and struggle to replicate a simple T or X shape built with a half-dozen building blocks. Few can balance a checkbook. These deficits generally erase about 35 points from whatever I.Q. the person would have inherited without the deletion. Since the average I.Q. is 100, this leaves most people with Williams with I.Q.?s in the 60s. Though some can hold simple jobs, they require assistance managing their lives.
The low I.Q., however, ignores two traits that define Williams more distinctly than do its deficits: an exuberant gregariousness and near-normal language skills. Williams people talk a lot, and they talk with pretty much anyone. They appear to truly lack social fear. Indeed, functional brain scans have shown that the brain?s main fear processor, the amygdala, which in most of us shows heightened activity when we see angry or worried faces, shows no reaction when a person with Williams views such faces. It?s as if they see all faces as friendly.
People with Williams tend to lack not just social fear but also social savvy. Lost on them are many meanings, machinations, ideas and intentions that most of us infer from facial expression, body language, context and stock phrasings. If you?re talking with someone with Williams syndrome and look at your watch and say: ?Oh, my, look at the time! Well it?s been awfully nice talking with you . . . ,? your conversational partner may well smile brightly, agree that ?this is nice? and ask if you?ve ever gone to Disney World. Because of this ? and because many of us feel uneasy with people with cognitive disorders, or for that matter with anyone profoundly unlike us ? people with Williams can have trouble deepening relationships. This saddens and frustrates them. They know no strangers but can claim few friends.
This paradox ? the urge to connect, the inability to fully do so ? sits at the center of the Williams puzzle, whether considered as a picture of human need (who hasn?t been shut out of a circle he?d like to join?) or, as a growing number of researchers are finding, a clue to the fundamental drives and tensions that shape social behavior. After being ignored for almost three decades, Williams has recently become one of the most energetically researched neurodevelopmental disability after autism, and it is producing more compelling insights. Autism, for starters, is a highly diverse ?spectrum disorder? with ill-defined borders, no identified mechanism and no clearly delineated genetic basis. Williams, in contrast, arises from a known genetic cause and produces a predictable set of traits and behaviors. It is ?an experiment of nature,? as the title of one paper puts it, perfect for studying not just how genes create intelligence and sociability but also how our powers of thought combine with our desire to bond to create complex social behavior ? a huge arena of interaction that largely determines our fates.
Julie R. Korenberg, a neurogeneticist at Cedars-Sinai Medical Center and at the University of California, Los Angeles, who has helped define the Williams deletion and explore its effects, believes the value of Williams syndrome in examining such questions is almost impossible to overstate. ?We?ve long figured that major behavioral traits rose in indirect fashion from a wide array of genes,? Korenberg says. ?But here we have this really tiny genetic deletion ? of the 20-some-odd genes missing, probably just 3 to 6 create the cognitive and social effects ? that reliably creates a distinctive behavioral profile. Williams isn?t just a fascinating mix of traits. It is the most compelling model available for studying the genetic bases of human behavior.?
Korenberg?s work is part of a diverse research effort on Williams that is illuminating a central dilemma of human existence: to survive we must relate and work with others, but we must also compete against them, lest we get left behind. It?s like the TV show ?Survivor?: we want to keep a place in the group ? we must ? and doing so requires not only charming others but also showing we can contribute to their success. This requires a finely calibrated display of smarts, savvy, grit and hustle. Show too little, and you?re voted off the island for being subpar. Show too much, and you?re ousted as a conniving threat.
Where is the right balance? A partial answer lies in the mix of skills, charms and deficiencies that is Williams syndrome.
Williams syndrome was first identified in 1961 by Dr. J. C. P. Williams of New Zealand. Williams, a cardiologist at Greenlane Hospital in Auckland, noticed that a number of the hospital?s young cardiac patients were small in stature, had elfin facial features and seemed friendly but in some ways were mentally slow. His published delineation of this syndrome put Dr. Williams on the map ? off which he promptly and mysteriously fell. Twice offered a position at the prestigious Mayo Clinic in Rochester, Minn., he twice failed to show, disappearing the second time, in the late ?60s, from London, his last known location, with the only trace an unclaimed suitcase later found in a luggage office.
The rarity of Williams syndrome ? about 1 in 7,500 people have it, compared with about 1 in 150 for autism or 1 in 800 for Down syndrome ? rendered it obscure. Unless they had the syndrome?s distinctive cardiovascular problems (which stem from the absence of the gene that makes blood vessels, heart valves and other tissue elastic and which even today limit the average lifespan of a person with Williams to around 50), most people with Williams were simply considered ?mentally retarded.?
This ended in the late 1980s, when a few researchers in the emerging field of cognitive neuroscience began to explore Williams. Among the most earnest was Ursula Bellugi, the director of the Laboratory for Cognitive Neuroscience at the Salk Institute for Biological Studies in La Jolla, Calif. Bellugi, who specializes in the neurobiology of language, was drawn to the linguistic strength that many Williamses displayed in the face of serious cognitive problems. The first person with Williams she met, in fact, came by referral from the linguist Noam Chomsky.
?The mother of that Williams teenager later connected me with two more, both in their teens,? Bellugi said. ?I didn?t have to talk to them long to realize something special was going on. Here they had these great cognitive deficits. Yet they spoke with the most ardent and delightful animation and color.?
To understand this uneven cognitive profile, Bellugi gave an array of language and cognitive tests to three groups: Williams children and teenagers, Down syndrome kids with similar I.Q.?s and developmentally average peers. ?We would do these warm-up interviews to get to know them, ask about their families,? said Bellugi, who, less than five feet tall and with a ready smile and an animated manner, is somewhat elfin and engagingly gregarious herself. ?Only, the Williams kids would turn the tables. They?d tell you how pretty you look or ask, ?Do you like opera?? They would ornament their answers in a way other kids didn?t. For instance, you?d ask an adolescent, ?What if you were a bird?? The Down kids said things like: ?I?m not a bird. I don?t fly.? The Williams teens would say: ?Good question! I?d fly through the air being free. If I saw a boy I?d land on his head and chirp.? ?
Bellugi found that this fanciful verbosity was accompanied by infectious affability. To measure it she developed a questionnaire and gave it to parents of Williams, Down and normal children. It asked about things like friendliness toward strangers, connections to familiar people, different social scenarios. At every age level, those with Williams scored significantly higher in sociability than those in the other groups. Having long studied the human capacity for language and its biological basis, Bellugi assumed that some extraordinary urge to use language drove this hypersociability: ?The language just seemed to be erupting out of them.?
Then she attended a meeting of Williams families that included infants and toddlers. ?That was about a year into my research project,? she says. ?The room was full of little ones ? babies, toddlers who weren?t speaking yet. And when I came in the room all the young children old enough to walk ran to the door to greet me. No clinging to Mom; they just broke away. And when I would talk to mothers holding infants ? literally babes in arms ? some of these babies would almost dive out of their mothers? arms to meet me.
?I knew then I was wrong. The language wasn?t driving the sociability. If anything, it was the other way around.?
Developmental psychologists sometimes call the social urge the ?drive to affiliate.? It seemed clear early on that the Williams deletion, which was definitively identified in the mid-1990s, either strengthened this drive or left it unfettered. But how do missing genes steer behavior toward gregariousness and engagement? How can a deletion heighten a trait rather than diminish it?